Fertilization Miss-Understood
Is the sperm truly a strong swimmer and powerful penetrator? Is the egg really a Damsel in Distress, a Sleeping Beauty, waiting to be activated and fertilized? Although it prides itself on its objectivity, there is an inherent gender bias in the sciences that influences the way research is conducted and interpreted. In particular, the passive-female and active-male stereotypes are embedded into the classic view of fertilization, and persists even in light of new evidence.
The earliest recorded scientific model of fertilization was developed by Aristotle (384-322 BCE). He argued that male ejaculate is the “seed” that implants itself into the menstrual “soil” of the female. He even described this “seed” generated by the male as containing the fetus itself: “By definition the male is that which is able to generate another…the female is that which is able to generate in itself and out of which comes into being the offspring previously existing in the generator” (Tuana). Aristotle also likened the process to a craftsman carving from a piece of wood, where the female provides the “raw material”, and the male “determines the function of the object to be produced and gives it its form” (Tuana). Incidentally, Aristotle’s reasoning had many inconsistencies, which only highlights the persistence of his underlying gender bias. These underlying assumptions are made clear when he states: “But the female, as female, is passive, and the male, as male, is active, and the principle of movement comes from him” (Tuana). This implies that females are passive and males are active by their very nature, and thus their contributions to conception follow accordingly.
Throughout history, the notion of female passivity has imbedded itself into the scientific model of fertilization. The same paradigm is propagated in the textbooks and research journals of the 20th century. The egg is described in passive terms: “eggs are “released”, or “shed” from the ovary […] until a sperm “penetrates” and “activates” them” (Tobach). Textbooks describe how “the egg is seen as large and passive. It does not move or journey, but passively “is transported”, “is swept”, or even “drifts” along the fallopian tube” (Martin). Despite growing evidence to the contrary, the passive female stereotype persists in our description of fertilization. The gender bias is often hidden in the language, even if the authors explicitly discuss the egg’s active role. In one article, the authors begin by saying “recent research suggests the almost heretical view that sperm and egg are mutually active partners” (Schatten). However, they later compare the egg to Sleeping Beauty: “[the egg] leaps from sleep to hubbub […] whose metabolic fires leap up and blaze at its fateful meeting with the sperm” (Schatten). Using this metaphor, the egg assumes the passive role of Sleeping Beauty, waiting for the Prince Charming sperm, even when the authors stated that the egg and sperm are mutually active partners! In such examples, the same traditional model is used even when it does not reflect the evidence, which goes to show how deeply rooted these biases are.
Results that challenge the notion of the active-male and passive-female face greater resistance before being accepted into the standard description of fertilization. In the 1983 edition of Developmental Biology (Gilbert), the sperm is said to be “able to travel long distances by whipping its flagellum”. Other textbooks use similar language to describe the sperm’s movement: “Their tails are ‘strong’ and efficiently powered. Together with the forces of ejaculation, they can ‘propel the semen into the deepest recesses of the vagina’” (Martin). The traditional model states that the sperm moves through the female system due to its own strength and speed. However, studies have shown that in fact the opposite is true: the female system moves the sperm: “The translocation of sperm from the vagina to the oviduct involves many processes that work at different times and places […] sperm motility (i.e. flagellar action) is probably a minor factor in getting the sperm to the oviduct […] rather, sperm appear to be translocated to the oviduct by the muscular activity of the uterus” (Schiebinger). The female reproductive system even brings the sperm to where fertilization is most likely: “the uterus and fallopian tubes of a woman will actively transport sperm to the dominant follicle” (Schiebinger). Although the female’s active role in sperm transport was well-documented since the 1930s, textbooks published 50 years later were still describing the sperm as self-propelled, with little to no mention of the female’s significant role in sperm motility.
Often the researchers who find evidence for female activity are prone to maintaining the passive stereotype in their interpretations and descriptions. In the traditional model, the egg passively waits for the sperm’s arrival, and the sperm breaks through the egg’s protective coat (the zona pellucida) as a result of its own strength and speed: “It was thought that the zona […] formed an impenetrable barrier. Sperm overcame the barrier by mechanically burrowing through, thrashing their tails and […] released digestive enzymes that chemically broke down the zona” (Martin). However, a recent study shows that sperm are actually weak swimmers. The study showed that the sperm’s “heads trashed from side to side ten times more vigorously than their bodies pushed forward (Freedman). As a result, rather than forcefully overcoming the egg’s barrier, “the flagellum produces forces on the head which act in directions tending to pull the sperm away from the zona” (Baltz). The results show that sperm are more prone to escaping the egg than penetrating it, when considering the sperm alone. In order for fertilization to be possible, the egg plays an active role by holding the sperm’s head in place on the zona. The egg’s role does not end there: certain bonds must be broken and formed at the right time for the sperm to successfully make it through the zona. The study suggests “that penetration of the zona pellucida by mammalian sperm must result from a highly organized and regulated sequence of events rather than being an inevitable result of the simple adhesion of a sperm with a beating flagellum to the zona” (Baltz). It is not possible for the sperm to bind to the egg and swim into it: the sperm can only cross the zona only when both sperm and egg are actively involved. The new findings show that the egg plays an equally active (and essential) role in bringing the sperm through the zona, yet this new evidence was resisted at first: “Not until August 1987, more than three years after the findings […] did these researchers reconceptualise the process to give the egg a more active role” (Martin). In the revised edition, the researchers refer to the interactive nature of the process: “sperm-zona bonds must capture the sperm”. However, they continue to attribute the process to the sperm’s activity alone: “Before reaching the egg plasma membrane, a sperm must first bind to the zona, reorient towards the egg, and penetrate the zona”. Even after these discoveries, it is difficult to abandon the active sperm and passive egg imagery.
In another case, the persistence of the passive stereotype is strikingly clear by the terms associated to structures on the egg and sperm. In light of new research, we know that the egg and sperm must “recognize” one another in order to proceed with fertilization. The egg and sperm have complimentary proteins on their cell surfaces, much like a lock and key. By convention, the engulfing protein (the “lock”) is called the “receptor”. The protruding protein (the “key”) is called the “ligand”, and is the molecule that initiates action on the “receptor”. Images of egg and sperm have shown that “the molecules on the sperm are proteins and have ‘pockets’” and the protein “ZP3 on the egg is a polymer of “keys”; many small knobs stick out” (Martin). Given conventional nomenclature, the sperm’s protein would be considered a receptor and the egg’s ZP3 protein would be considered the ligand. Yet, a study that isolated the ZP3 protein referred to it as a “sperm receptor” and described the sperm’s protein as an “egg-binding protein”! They describe: (emphasis added)
“The basic process begins when many sperm first attach loosely and then bind tenaciously to receptors on the surface of the egg’s thick outer coat, the zona pellucida. Each sperm, which has a large number of egg-binding proteins on its surface, binds to many sperm receptors on the egg […] a site on each of the egg-binding proteins fits a complimentary site on a sperm receptor, much as a key fits a lock” (Wassarman)
In the description, the egg is assigned the passive role of “receiving” even though it contradicts conventional nomenclature of proteins. On the other hand, the sperm is given the active role of “binding”, even though its protein is technically a receptor. The egg’s protein is also referred to as the “lock” and the sperm’s as the “key”, despite their obvious shapes. There are many other ways of describing this process, one being the joining of two complimentary proteins. Yet, the description chosen is a poor reflection of their findings. This illustrates how deeply the notions of active-male and passive-female are ingrained in our interpretation of fertilization.
The role of science is to provide an objective explanation or expression of the true nature of our world. For this to be possible, science must be founded on truth and must adhere to sound reason and evidence. Gender biases and gender stereotypes that influence scientific description undermine the very tenets of scientific inquiry. The “passive female” stereotype is used to describe fertilization even when scientific research proves that the female plays a very active role. Even once the evidence is known, there is much resistance to changing the model of fertilization. Holding onto old gender stereotypes limits our understanding of the truth and inhibits scientific inquiry. Our gender preconceptions influence which questions we ask ourselves, and as a result, which areas are explored in research. When the sperm has traditionally been revered for its strength and speed, it is no wonder the study examining its weakness was so recent. In the experiment, the most sophisticated technology used was a manometer, which has been around since the 1800s. What took researchers so long was to ask the question; to challenge the notion of the “macho” sperm. If our view of fertilization states that the male contribution is active, whereas the female contribution is passive, we will never investigate the many active processes of the female.
Historically, the notion of the “passive female” in fertilization may be a rationalization for female inferiority. When Aristotle suggested females play a passive role in reproduction, it was guided by the underlying assumption that women are inferior to men. Even after the flaws in his model were revealed, the same gender roles lingered for millennia. When gender stereotypes are projected onto scientific descriptions, it gives the stereotypes scientific credibility. In the case of the egg and the sperm, the active-male and passive-female stereotypes appear to be biologically founded, whereas they are more likely a projection of the observer’s underlying gender biases. The stereotypes originally used to describe sexual and societal roles at a “macro” level are also being used to describe the egg and sperm’s roles at the “micro” level. When the sperm’s strength and speed is symbolically linked to male potency, the “macro” and “micro” male roles are used interchangeably. Likewise, when the egg is described as playing a passive role in fertilization, this might translate to the assumption that women play a passive role in conception. Using gender stereotypes in describing fertilization risks being extrapolated to apply to gender roles in society.
In light of new scientific evidence, the passive-female and active-male stereotypes can no longer be used as accurate metaphors for fertilization. Nevertheless, these gender stereotypes have a lasting effect on scientific research and interpretation. In particular, even scientific findings that contradict the passive-active stereotypes are influenced by them, such as in the descriptions of sperm motility, of how the sperm passes through the zona, and the naming of surface proteins. This gender bias has embedded itself into the scientific culture, where even the researchers heading these studies, perhaps unknowingly, keep the stereotypes alive. Using the gender stereotypes in describing fertilization compromises scientific objectivity and reinforces gender misconceptions on the human scale. Although the strong-swimmer sperm and the Damsel in Distress egg makes for a good narrative, there may be other compelling stories yet to be told.
Works Cited
Baltz, J., Katz, D. & Cone, R. Mechanics of Sperm-Egg Interaction at the Zona Pellucida. Biophysical Journal: Vol. 54 (October 1988). Web.
Freedman, David. New Theory on How The Aggressive Egg Attracts Sperm. Discover Magazine (June 1992). Web.
Martin, Emily. The Egg and the Sperm: How Science has Constructed a Romance Based on Stereotypical Male-Female Roles. The University of Chicago Press: Signs, Vol. 16, No. 3 (Spring, 1991), pp. 485–501. Web.
Schatten, Gerald & Schatten, Heide. The Energetic Egg. The Sciences: Vol. 23, Issue 5 (October 1983). pp.28-35. Web.
Schiebinger, L., Klinge, I., Sánchez de Madariaga, I., Schraudner, M., & Stefanick, M. (Eds.) (2011-2013). Textbooks: Rethinking Language and Visual Representations. Gendered Innovations in Science, Health & Medicine, Engineering, and Environment. Web.
Short, R.V. The Discovery of the Ovaries. Stanford Academic Press: 1977. Web.
Tobach, Ethel & Rosoff, Betty. Challenging Racism and Sexism: Alternatives to Genetic Explanations. The Feminist Press at The City University of New York: 1994. pp. 15-19. eBook.
Tuana, Nancy. The Weaker Seed: The Sexist Bias of Reproductive Theory. Hypatia. Vol. 3, No. 1 (Spring 1988). pp.35-59. eBook.
Wassarman, Paul M. Fertilization in Mammals. Scientific American: Vol. 259, No. 6 (December 1988). pp.78-84. Web.
Comments
Miranda Samoluk
November 24, 2014This is a fascinating article that reveals the truth behind conception. As much as we, as a society, try to avoid gender stereotypes (especially recently) it’s undeniable that these biases are embedded in our minds. It’s really quite alarming how much we have let these stereotypes affect our basic understanding of the world around us and of our respective roles in it. I had no idea that I was misunderstanding basic biology to this extent. This is just a subject I’ve never thought to question, and apparently, scientist haven’t questioned it too much either. C’mon scientists, get it right. Nonetheless, I’m thankful to have yet another reason to take pride in my gender.
afilippi
November 27, 2014This was such an interesting article to read. I, like many others, had no idea there was such a misunderstanding in conception. I like how the author refered to the egg’s proteins as the lock and the sperm’s as the key. It makes the scientific concepts a lot easier to understand when given everyday examples. The article also proves that society has always thought of women as inferior. It says since the 1930s scientifics have proof of the egg and sperm being both active in the conception but yet the sperm is still said to be the only active part.
Leen Yamani
November 28, 2014I absolutely love this. There are some very poignant gender biases in our society even now, and to have them influence our sciences is a huge misstep.
I’m glad I could read this, because I had no idea that this was a misconception and not the truth.
Hannah Gerber
November 30, 2014Reading this article, I realize how unaware I was regarding the roots of gender roles we so naturally resort to. I think we generally tend not to question the issues you surface in this article because “it’s science.” But as you brought up, the sexism wasn’t necessarily presented in the facts, but in their contradictory delivery. This touches upon how sexism is integrated into our behaviour, our articulation, even if it contradicts the truth or subject of what we fail to express. Your extensive research is impressive and its range supports your argument strongly. Brava!
Sabrina Shemie
December 1, 2014The perspective on conception presented makes this article very interesting as its ideas challenge those of many scientists. We are often unaware of the bias that is found in everything we read or hear about, including science. Gender roles are often believed to have no impact on the “facts” that scientific research provides us with. However, this article is a perfect demonstration of the effect of gender roles on our interpretation of scientific information. It teaches us to consider the various biases that may be found in information originating from even the most reliable sources. Prior to reading this article, I, too, was convinced of the egg’s role as a Damsel in Distress and of the sperm’s role as its Prince Charming. This article revolutionized the way I see these roles and led me to the realization that the egg’s importance in fertilization is much greater than what we would expect.
Tamara Nazywalskyjj
December 15, 2014Dear Damsel, what a provoking, and insightful piece.
Not only have made a new kind of break in gender gaps, but your perspective on female and male interaction on a fundamental level is followed up with facts.
I guess it’s easy to assume that we’re all kind of Sleeping Ladies just waiting and yearning for a man to come along and give purpose to our ovaries. But that sort of assumption is dated and should stay in the past, what everyone should be hearing is your new twist on the stereotypes that come swimming along with men and women, society, and reproduction.
Super fascinating, super insightful, super good read, and something worth sharing.
You have to be registered and logged in in order to post comments!